Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.

What you'll learn to do: describe latent learning and observational learning

Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.
Classical and operant conditioning are responsible for a good bit of the behaviors we learn and develop, but certainly there are other things we learn simply through observation and thought. Latent learning is a form of learning that occurs without any obvious reinforcement of the behavior or associations that are learned.

According to Albert Bandura, learning can occur by watching others and then modeling what they do or say. This is known as observational learning. There are specific steps in the process of modeling that must be followed if learning is to be successful. These steps include attention, retention, reproduction, and motivation. Through modeling, Bandura has shown that children learn many things both good and bad simply by watching their parents, siblings, and others. What have you learned by observation?

Learning Objectives

  • Explain latent learning and cognitive maps
  • Describe Edward Tolman’s experiment on latent learning


Although strict behaviorists such as Skinner and Watson refused to believe that cognition (such as thoughts and expectations) plays a role in learning, another behaviorist, Edward C. Tolman, had a different opinion. Tolman’s experiments with rats demonstrated that organisms can learn even if they do not receive immediate reinforcement (Tolman & Honzik, 1930; Tolman, Ritchie, & Kalish, 1946).

Latent learning is a form of learning that is not immediately expressed in an overt response. It occurs without any obvious reinforcement of the behavior or associations that are learned. Latent learning is not readily apparent to the researcher because it is not shown behaviorally until there is sufficient motivation. This type of learning broke the constraints of behaviorism, which stated that processes must be directly observable and that learning was the direct consequence of conditioning to stimuli.

In the experiments, Tolman placed hungry rats in a maze with no reward for finding their way through it. He also studied a comparison group that was rewarded with food at the end of the maze. As the unreinforced rats explored the maze, they developed a cognitive map: a mental picture of the layout of the maze (Figure 1). After 10 sessions in the maze without reinforcement, food was placed in a goal box at the end of the maze. As soon as the rats became aware of the food, they were able to find their way through the maze quickly, just as quickly as the comparison group, which had been rewarded with food all along. This is known as latent learning: learning that occurs but is not observable in behavior until there is a reason to demonstrate it.

Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.
Figure 1. Psychologist Edward Tolman found that rats use cognitive maps to navigate through a maze. Have you ever worked your way through various levels on a video game? You learned when to turn left or right, move up or down. In that case you were relying on a cognitive map, just like the rats in a maze. (credit: modification of work by "FutUndBeidl"/Flickr)

Latent learning also occurs in humans. Children may learn by watching the actions of their parents but only demonstrate it at a later date, when the learned material is needed. For example, suppose that Ravi’s dad drives him to school every day. In this way, Ravi learns the route from his house to his school, but he’s never driven there himself, so he has not had a chance to demonstrate that he’s learned the way. One morning Ravi’s dad has to leave early for a meeting, so he can’t drive Ravi to school. Instead, Ravi follows the same route on his bike that his dad would have taken in the car. This demonstrates latent learning. Ravi had learned the route to school, but had no need to demonstrate this knowledge earlier.

Everyday Connection: This Place Is Like a Maze

Have you ever gotten lost in a building and couldn’t find your way back out? While that can be frustrating, you’re not alone. At one time or another we’ve all gotten lost in places like a museum, hospital, or university library. Whenever we go someplace new, we build a mental representation—or cognitive map—of the location, as Tolman’s rats built a cognitive map of their maze. However, some buildings are confusing because they include many areas that look alike or have short lines of sight. Because of this, it’s often difficult to predict what’s around a corner or decide whether to turn left or right to get out of a building. Psychologist Laura Carlson (2010) suggests that what we place in our cognitive map can impact our success in navigating through the environment. She suggests that paying attention to specific features upon entering a building, such as a picture on the wall, a fountain, a statue, or an escalator, adds information to our cognitive map that can be used later to help find our way out of the building.

Watch this video to learn more about Laura Carlson’s studies on cognitive maps and navigation in buildings.

Tolman's Experiment

Edward Tolmanwas studying traditional trial-and-error learning when he realized that some of his research subjects (rats) actually knew more than their behavior initially indicated. In one of Tolman’s classic experiments, he observed the behavior of three groups of hungry rats that were learning to navigate mazes.

The first group always received a food reward at the end of the maze, so the payoff for learning the maze was real and immediate. The second group never received any food reward, so there was no incentive to learn to navigate the maze effectively. The third group was like the second group for the first 10 days, but on the 11th day, food was now placed at the end of the maze.

As you might expect when considering the principles of conditioning, the rats in the first group quickly learned to negotiate the maze, while the rats of the second group seemed to wander aimlessly through it. The rats in the third group, however, although they wandered aimlessly for the first 10 days, quickly learned to navigate to the end of the maze as soon as they received food on day 11. By the next day, the rats in the third group had caught up in their learning to the rats that had been rewarded from the beginning. It was clear to Tolman that the rats that had been allowed to experience the maze, even without any reinforcement, had nevertheless learned something, and Tolman called this latent learning. Latent learning is to learning that is not reinforced and not demonstrated until there is motivation to do so. Tolman argued that the rats had formed a “cognitive map” of the maze but did not demonstrate this knowledge until they received reinforcement.

Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.
Figure 1. The maze. As you can see from the map, the maze had lots of doors and curtains to make it difficult for the rats to master. The blue marks represent doors that swung both directions, which prevented the rat from seeing most of the junctions as it approached. This forced the rat to go through the door to discover what was on the other side. The green forms show curtains. These hung down and prevented the rat from getting a long distance perspective and it also meant that they could not see a wall at the end of a wrong turn until they had already made a choice and moved in that direction. The rat was always in a small area, unable to see beyond the next door or curtain, so learning the maze was a formidable task.




Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.


Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.


Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.

Work It out

Your task here is to predict what is going to happen on Trial 12 for the “no food until Trial 11” group.

Option A: Notice that this result is the same as the “no food on any trial” group. So, if you choose option A, you think that they will not act differently now than they acted on the first 11 trials and they will continue to make a lot of wrong turns.

Option B: This option suggests that they are now motivated to learn the path to the food, but that they will do so in small steps, just as we have seen for all three groups up to this point. Option B says that they are moving in the direction of the “food on every trial” group, but that it will take some extra learning to get there.

Option C: This option says that they already know the path to the food and, now that they are motivated to get there, they will show that they already know just as much as the “food on every trial” group. Their performance on Trial 12 will be the same as the low-error performance of the “food on every trial” group.

Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.



Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.


Latent, or implicit, learning is learning that is not immediately reflected in behavior.

Glossary

cognitive map: mental picture of the layout of the environment

latent learning: learning that occurs, but it may not be evident until there is a reason to demonstrate it

Licenses and Attributions

CC licensed content, Shared previously

  • Latent Learning: Learning Before Doing. Provided by: Open Learning Initiative. Located at: https://oli.cmu.edu/jcourse/webui/login/required.do?redirect=%2Fjcourse%2Fworkbook%2Factivity%2Fpage%3Fcontext%3Ddf3e71c60a0001dc051db622d622b3f7&. Project: Psychology. License: CC BY-NC-SA: Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
  • Operant Conditioning and Observational Learning. Authored by: OpenStax College. Located at: https://openstax.org/books/psychology/pages/6-3-operant-conditioning. License: CC BY: Attribution. License terms: Download for free at http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]
  • Latent Learning. Authored by: Boundless. Located at: https://www.coursehero.com/study-guides/boundless-psychology/. License: CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Traquair House Maze. Authored by: marsroverdriver. Located at: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Traquair_House_Maze.jpg. License: CC BY: Attribution

What is a true statement about latent learning?

Identify a true statement about latent learning. It occurs without prior reinforcement. that provides reinforcement for a response only if an unvarying time period has elapsed, making overall rates of response relatively low.

Which of the following is an example of latent learning?

In psychology, latent learning refers to knowledge that only becomes clear when a person has an incentive to display it. For example, a child might learn how to complete a math problem in class, but this learning is not immediately apparent.

Is latent learning classical conditioning?

Understanding Latent Learning It's different than classical or operant conditioning, which are techniques that stimulate or reinforce specific behaviors with rewards. Latent learning has no immediate benefits but remains in a subject's mind until they need to demonstrate it.

When the delayed consequences of behavior are punishment and the immediate?

3) Immediate vs. Delayed Reinforcement and Punishment: When the delayed consequences of behavior are punishing and the immediate consequences are reinforcing, the immediate consequences usually win --> even if immediate are minor and delayed are major.